intTypePromotion=1
zunia.vn Tuyển sinh 2024 dành cho Gen-Z zunia.vn zunia.vn
ADSENSE

Động từ trạng thái và tình thái trong các bài báo kinh tế tiếng Anh

Chia sẻ: ViKakashi2711 ViKakashi2711 | Ngày: | Loại File: PDF | Số trang:7

42
lượt xem
5
download
 
  Download Vui lòng tải xuống để xem tài liệu đầy đủ

Bài viết này cố gắng nghiên cứu xa hơn về biểu hiện phương thức và động từ tình thái bằng cách tập trung vào phân tích các phát ngôn tình thái và động từ trạng thái sử dụng trong các văn bản kinh tế được lựa chọn. Bài viết này dựa trên nghiên cứu 15 bài báo kinh tế tiếng Anh.

Chủ đề:
Lưu

Nội dung Text: Động từ trạng thái và tình thái trong các bài báo kinh tế tiếng Anh

NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI v<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> ĐỘNG TỪ TRẠNG THÁI<br /> VÀ TÌNH THÁI<br /> TRONG CÁC BÀI BÁO<br /> KINH TẾ TIẾNG ANH<br /> PHẠM THỊ THANH THÙY<br /> Đại học Kinh tế Quốc dân<br /> <br /> <br /> 1. INTRODUCTION<br /> <br /> TÓM TẮT Modality and transitivity has been the subject of<br /> linguistic studies and research. Modal expressions<br /> Trong lĩnh vực ngôn ngữ học, “nghĩa tình thái” là<br /> allow us to talk about a certain state of affair that may<br /> một thuật ngữ liên quan đến thái độ của người<br /> never occur in the actual world. Particularly, modality is<br /> nói đối với những gì đang được nói. Động từ trạng<br /> thái là một nhóm động từ đưa ra yêu cầu hoặc đề a facet of illocutionary force, signaled by grammatical<br /> xuất trong một trường hợp nhất định. Nghĩa tình devices that expresses (i) the illocutionary point or<br /> thái và động từ trạng thái trong các văn bản kinh general intent of a speaker or (ii) a speaker’s degree<br /> tế đại diện cho một lĩnh vực nghiên cứu nhỏ về of commitment to the expressed proposition’s<br /> khả năng ngữ dụng học (khả năng thực tế). Phần believability, obligatory, desirability, or reality. In the<br /> lớn sự chú ý tập trung vào lý thuyết, tập trung linguistics literature, it is widely acknowledged that<br /> vào các ví dụ trực quan và vào các ví dụ từ những modal expressions may be used to communicate<br /> ngữ cảnh thực tế. Do đó, bài viết này sẽ xem two great clusters of meanings: (i) epistemic modal<br /> xét các khái niệm trong một số chiến lược rộng<br /> meanings dealing with the possibility or necessity<br /> hơn cho việc thay đổi lực ngôn trung dựa trên<br /> of an inference drawn from available evidence, and<br /> sự phân tích nguồn đáng tin cậy từ các bài báo<br /> nghiên cứu kinh tế. Theo đó, bài viết này cố gắng (ii) deontic modal meanings concerning with the<br /> nghiên cứu xa hơn về biểu hiện phương thức và necessity or possibility of acts performed by morally<br /> động từ tình thái bằng cách tập trung vào phân responsible agents, e.g. obligation and permission<br /> tích các phát ngôn tình thái và động từ trạng thái (Lyons, 1977; Palmer, 1986, 1990). The paper attempts<br /> sử dụng trong các văn bản kinh tế được lựa chọn. to answer the following questions: (1) Are modal<br /> Bài viết này dựa trên nghiên cứu 15 bài báo kinh verbs or non-modals used in economic text more<br /> tế tiếng Anh. Thông tin trong 15 bài báo kinh tế passive or active? (2) Which kind of modal verbs are<br /> tiếng Anh bao gồm toàn bộ bài báo không tính frequently used in mental process? And (3) Is there<br /> phần tóm tắt vì tác giả cho rằng ngôn ngữ của any relationship between transitivity and modality?<br /> phần tóm lược thuộc một thể loại khác so với các<br /> phần còn lại trong một bài báo khoa học.<br /> The study is based on a corpus of research articles<br /> Từ khóa: bài báo nghiên cứu kinh tế, động từ drawn from English economic field. The corpus is<br /> khuyết thiếu, động từ trạng thái, tình thái made up of 15 economic research articles from The<br /> Economic Journal (5 articles); Journal of Economic<br /> Issues (3 articles); Quarterly Journal of Business and<br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> Số 3 - 9/2016 63<br /> v NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI<br /> <br /> <br /> Economics (2 articles); The American Economic Review Since there are indefinitely many ways of drawing lines<br /> (3 articles); Australian Economic (2 articles). on purely semantic grounds, we must inquire which<br /> have systematic repercussions in the grammar. We see<br /> Only articles written by native speakers are chosen a good illustration in the criteria to show why mental<br /> in order to avoid possible interference from other processes (i.e. ‘sensing’) and material processes (i.e.<br /> languages. The articles which make up the corpus ‘doing’) constitute distinct grammatical categories.<br /> relate to the macro and micro-economics issues. ‘Mental process’ is distinct from ‘material process’ in:<br /> Based on the structure of the active English verb: (a) having as ‘participants’ a ‘human sensor’ endowed<br /> with consciousness and a ‘Phenomenon’ (as in “I like<br /> (modality) (perfect aspect) (progressive aspect) the quiet”), which cannot be equated with Actor and<br /> tense Goal in a material process (as in “the lion caught the<br /> tourist”); (b) being ‘representable’ as two-way or ‘bi-<br /> in which brackets indicate optional elements, all directional’ (as in “Mary liked the gift” versus “the gift<br /> verbs which fit into the above modality slot are pleased Mary”); (c) being a ‘Fact’ or a representation<br /> chosen as modal verbs. The study excludes ought, ready packaged, (as in “Jane saw that the stars had<br /> which requires the inclusion of the particle to, and come out”) as well as ‘a Thing’, or ‘a phenomenon of<br /> have to, which, in any case, has syntactic features our experience’ (as in “Jane saw the stars”); and (d)<br /> which distinguish it from the modal verbs. having as unmarked simple present tense (as in “I see<br /> the stars”), whereas the material process has ‘present<br /> 2. REVIEW OF LITERATURE<br /> in present’ (as in “they are building a house”).<br /> 2.1. Transitivity<br /> 2.2. Modality<br /> Halliday’s (1970) way of classifying processes<br /> From various points of view, despite an enormous<br /> instructively shows his balancing the plausible with the amount of work done on the analysis of modality in<br /> technical as well as the semantic with the grammatical. languages, it is still not easy to identify what modality<br /> Mental processes have the principal subtypes of actually means (Palmer, 1986).<br /> perception (“seeing, hearing”, etc.), affection (“liking,<br /> fearing”, etc.), and cognition (“thinking, knowing, According to Quirk et al. (1972), modality is used<br /> understanding”, etc.). Meanwhile, material processes as a cover term for different types of modification<br /> are divided into dispositive (“doing to”) and creative the speaker can express towards a state of affairs<br /> (“bringing about”), each of which may be either contained in the proposition. In this usage, it is<br /> concrete or abstract. identified with the speaker’s comment or evaluation<br /> on the representational content of the utterance.<br /> According to Halliday, every language accommodates “Attitude” and “opinion” in this sense have nothing to<br /> in its grammar a number of distinct ways of being and do with the emotional state that a person undergoes<br /> English has intensive (i.e., ‘a relation of sameness’) (as such as “like”, “love”, “hate”…. Moreover, modality<br /> in “Tony is the leader”), circumstantial (as in “the fair is excludes factual evaluation from its domain. Linguistic<br /> on a Thursday”), and possessive (as in “Kate has a car”). discussions witness three kinds of modality: epistemic,<br /> Each of these three comes in two modes: attributive deontic and dynamic. Epistemic modality deals with<br /> which has the functions of ‘attribute and carrier’ (as in the degree of the speaker’s commitment to the truth<br /> “Sarah is wise”), and identifying which has identified of the proposition expressed and thus indicates some<br /> and identifier (as in “tomorrow is the tenth”). Only degree of certainty (as in “He must know the answer of<br /> identifying clauses are reversible and have a passive the question”) or uncertainty (as in “He might know the<br /> (as in “Tony plays the leader” and “the leader is played answer of the question”).<br /> by Tony”); but attributives do not (as in “the fair lasts all<br /> day” but not “all day is lasted by the fair”), because an Deontic modality is concerned with the necessity or<br /> attribute is not a participant and so cannot become a possibility of acts performed by morally responsible<br /> subject of a sentence. agents and thus mainly covers such notions as<br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> 64 Số 3 - 9/2016<br /> NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI v<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> obligation (e.g. “You must take care of the child until perception (reaction) may not then increase in the firm’s<br /> 31st Dec.”) and permission (e.g. “You may check in systematic risk level.<br /> books for 2 weeks).<br /> Relation processes are either attributive, where a<br /> According to Von Wright (1951), dynamic modality is qualitative attribute is assigned to the subject: (6)<br /> introduced for cases where circumstances in the real … increasing price in… may be risky…or identifying,<br /> world make possible or necessary the actualization of where the process serves to define the identity of the<br /> a state of affairs. Dynamic modality includes two sub- subject: (7)…both financial and non-financial private<br /> categories: subject-oriented, where the circumstances benefits of control… which may create incentives for<br /> are characteristics of the subject (as in “She can’t think corporate insiders…<br /> on her own”); and neutral, where the circumstances<br /> are general conditions holding in the world (as in Only four positions in the network introduced by<br /> “that career might be the most wanted job in the next Halliday (1985) accept a selection between active<br /> few years”). However, dynamic modality is not as clear and passive voice; they are the causative, transitive,<br /> as it seems not to relate to the speaker. Therefore, mental, and identifying processes. Table 1 shows the<br /> linguists always mention to two broad kinds of distribution of modal verbs in economic research<br /> modality only: epistemic and deontic, and many of articles from the corpus by process and distinguishes<br /> them use “root modality” or “non-epistemic” modality between active and passive where appropriate.<br /> as a term covering both deontic and dynamic cases<br /> (Coates, 1983). Table 1: Distribution by process and active, passive<br /> voice (data from writer’s research)<br /> 3. MODALITY AND TRANSITIVITY IN ENGLISH<br /> ECONOMIC RESEARCH ARTICLES Processes and Voices Number of Frequency<br /> modal verbs<br /> Data from the corpus show that while 31% of finite Active 3 1%<br /> verbs are passive, 41% of modal verbs occur in the Causative<br /> Passive 3 1%<br /> passive form. Based on a simple transitivity network Non-causative 9 4%<br /> introduced by Berry (1975) and Halliday (1985), Middle 17 7%<br /> passivization and transitivity are considered. Active 21 9%<br /> Transitive<br /> Passive 26 11%<br /> In economic research articles, all processes are either<br /> Intransitive 2 1%<br /> mental or relational. Material processes are either<br /> Active 34 14%<br /> unrestricted or restricted. In other word, the number Mental<br /> of inherent participants is fixed (restricted) or not Passive 72 29%<br /> (unrestricted). Unrestricted processes may occur Attributive 49 20%<br /> with two participants, and thus be causative: (1)… Active 14 6%<br /> Identifying<br /> corporate insiders must promote risk-taking decisions Passive 2 1%<br /> to capitalize... or, with a single participant, in which<br /> case they are non-causative: (2)… petrol price will From the table 1, it is seen that mental process<br /> similarly increase. Restricted processes may be accounts for the largest number of cases (106 or<br /> restricted to a single participant, and thus be middle: 43%), followed by the material (81 or 34%). Relational<br /> (3) When we use the three years at the same time, N in process takes up 65 cases (26%). As has been said,<br /> that model can be bigger than the N of each year. 41% of the modal examples occur in the passive form.<br /> However, the result shown in the study expresses a<br /> Where restricted processes are restricted to two misleading, in the sense that in four of the process<br /> participants (effective process), both participants may types (non-causative, middle, intransitive and<br /> be expressed giving the transitive process: (4) … they attributive) the active/passive choice is not available.<br /> [insiders] can create an adversarial relationship among These are processes occurring uniquely in the active<br /> partners…or only one participant may be expressed form; no passive correlate exists. Of the other types,<br /> giving the intransitive process: (5) A negative market causative has few examples, but those that do occur<br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> Số 3 - 9/2016 65<br /> v NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI<br /> <br /> <br /> are evenly divided between active and passive. The transitive cases are also fairly evenly divided, with a slight<br /> bias towards passive (55%). The identifying cases are not many but are more so than causative. Passive is rare in<br /> this case with only 12% of the cases.<br /> <br /> From the results, there seems to be some slight correlation between modality and mental process: 42% of the<br /> modal verbs in the corpus are mental processes, compared with 33% material and 25% relational processes.<br /> In the material process category, active and passive examples seem equally probable; in the mental process<br /> category, there seems to be a strong tendency towards the passive. On the other hand, passive seems extremely<br /> rare in the relational process.<br /> <br /> It is reasonable that there are some links between modality and mental process. According to Halliday (1985),<br /> mental process deal with the human appreciation of the world, it may be well that this is a situation where<br /> the normative economists tend to hedge their bets, leaving themselves an escape route rather than making<br /> unqualified statements. The qualification provided by the modal would then allow the readers to follow the line<br /> of reasoning suggested. McCloskey (1985) has shown that when talking about the supposed errors of others,<br /> normative economists tend to do so in a non-specific way when speaking in a formal situation, as opposed<br /> to the informal situations where they tend to be much more specific and assertive. Therefore, it would seem<br /> reasonable that such a formal situation as in research articles will lead economists to be less assertive to hedge<br /> their bets, on points that might be open to disagreement.<br /> <br /> Table 2 indicates the distribution of the various modal verbs in the corpus occurring in mental processes.<br /> <br /> Table 2: Distribution of modal verbs in mental processes (data from writer’s research)<br /> <br /> Modal Active Passive Total Mental<br /> Verbs<br /> Number Frequency Number Frequency Number Frequency<br /> Will 3 9% 5 7% 8 8%<br /> Would 7 21% 3 4% 10 10%<br /> Shall - - - - - -<br /> Should 3 9% 3 4% 6 6%<br /> Can 15 42% 34 47% 49 46%<br /> Could 1 3% 4 6% 5 5%<br /> May 4 12% 15 21% 19 18%<br /> Might - - 6 8% 6 6%<br /> Must 1 3% 2 3% 3 3%<br /> <br /> Table 3 shows the frequency of the various modal verbs by process and by voice. The modal verbs are ordered<br /> according to their high frequency.<br /> <br /> Table 3: The distribution of the various modal verbs by process and by voice (data from writer’s research)<br /> <br /> Modal Verbs Material Mental Relational<br /> Active Passive Active Passive Active Passive<br /> Can 6 7 15 34 4 -<br /> May 10 5 4 15 20 1<br /> Would 12 4 7 3 15 1<br /> Will 11 1 3 5 7 -<br /> Should 1 4 3 3 8 -<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> 66 Số 3 - 9/2016<br /> NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI v<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> Could 6 4 1 4 2 -<br /> Must 7 2 1 2 4 -<br /> Might - 1 - 6 3 -<br /> <br /> The above tables indicate that can makes up almost half of the modal verbs occurring. This is true for both the<br /> active and passive cases, with 42% and 47% respectively in mental processes. May is also accounts for a high<br /> ratio. These two modal verbs can and may will further be considered in the next section.<br /> <br /> 3.1. Mental process and passive May and Can<br /> <br /> Huddleston (1971) claims that there are six uses of the epistemic may as follow:<br /> <br /> 1. Qualified generalization: x is true for at least some members of the group but not necessarily any particular<br /> individual.<br /> <br /> 2. Exhaustive disjunction: x is at least one of the attributes proposed.<br /> <br /> 3. Uncertainty: the truth of x is not certain (i.e. possibility).<br /> <br /> 4. Concession: whether x is true or not, y is the case.<br /> <br /> 5. Legitimacy: x is legitimate, valid.<br /> <br /> 6. Ability: people are able to do x.<br /> <br /> The epistemic can has the same uses as may with the exclusion of the fourth use: concession. Besides,<br /> Huddleston seems to imply that there is little difference between may and can. Palmer (1974) when mentioning<br /> English verbs also shares the same idea links can to the notion of non-assertion. Of the notions suggested by<br /> Huddleston (1971), those of the uncertainly/possibility, legitimacy and ability seem to cover all of the examples<br /> found in the sample.<br /> <br /> The examples of passive can in the mental process are never of a deontic type. All of the examples can be<br /> classified as epistemic in some forms. To the extent that these can be divided into an ability type and a<br /> possibility type. For example: (8) Hence, it can be argued that the nature of and changes in corporate governance<br /> have potential implications for the firm’s riskiness; (9) The differences in securities underwritten by commercial banks<br /> (or their affiliates) can be neglected.<br /> <br /> However, a considerable number of the examples are not easily classified as being one rather than the other,<br /> and seem to combine aspects of both possible interpretations, or indeed to be neutral as to which one should<br /> be selected.<br /> <br /> The examples of passive, may in the mental process are also exclusively of the epistemic type, i.e. indicating<br /> some form of possibility. (10)… the present work may be considered an extension of Walter’s analysis…<br /> <br /> Here the majority of the cases of passive may in mental process (15 out of 17 cases) are examples of the legitimacy<br /> use: (11)… without controlling for other factors that will affect this spread, no strong conclusions may be drawn from<br /> these univariate results.<br /> <br /> The ability category of may can also be seen in the economic research articles (here again some cases are not<br /> easy to categorize).<br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> Số 3 - 9/2016 67<br /> v NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI<br /> <br /> <br /> To the extent that can and may cover similar semantic While the other examples express ability, for example:<br /> areas in similar proportions, it might seem that the (18)…lacking suitable data we cannot quantify the loss<br /> “virtually no difference” approach is justified, and it from that economic decision.<br /> is true that in many cases substituting may for can<br /> or vice versa would make “virtually” no difference. Some examples are neutral as to an ability or<br /> However, in this example, “can” does not lend legitimacy interpretation as (19) We can check<br /> themselves to this ploy. (12)… it can be shown that whether the loss Profile B is plausible…; (20) We may<br /> the procedure… is reasonable… The substitution of conduct further tests to clarify the effects of time on the<br /> may in example (13)… in the United States, the capital perception of a conflict of interest.<br /> market needs of smaller firms may be ignored …places<br /> the reasonableness much more in the domain of The above corpus examples then present two basic<br /> possibility. This seems to go against Palmer’s use of differences between active may and active can in<br /> non-assertion as a distinguishing feature of epistemic mental process. Firstly, may, but not can, occurs<br /> can. Both may and can express a procedure which more frequently with untypical animacy; whereas<br /> is considered legitimate. However, in the case of may expresses primarily uncertainty and secondarily<br /> can, this legitimacy is considered to be the only one legitimacy, can expresses primarily legitimacy and<br /> available in the present state of our knowledge, and it secondarily ability. The numbers are too small to<br /> is used until it is shown to be less adequate than some warrant extrapolation, but it would be interesting to<br /> other procedures. This interpretation is supported see if these results are confirmed in a larger sample.<br /> by the fact that in many cases there seems to be<br /> 4. CONCLUSION<br /> little difference, or at least only marginal difference,<br /> between the use of can and a non-modal sentence. In conclusion, the study presents some observations<br /> concerning transitivity and modality in economic<br /> 3.2. Mental Process and Active May and Can<br /> research articles. It can be concluded that there is a<br /> Mental process, by definition, typically requires an series of relationships between processes (transitivity)<br /> intelligent agent. Economic discourse tends to avoid and modality. These are manifested in the form of<br /> the use of human agent subjects. It might expect then tendencies, in some cases particularly strong, for<br /> that active mental process would be rare in this type particular processes to have strong collocations with<br /> of document. As far as may is concerned, this is true. In specific forms of modal choice in economic writing.<br /> addition, example with human subject is rarely found. Transitivity is then a significant parameter in the<br /> Subject in most of the cases might be called “untypical analysis of modality in economic research articles.<br /> animacy” (the term used by Berry (1975)); that is, in this<br /> Because of the scope of a seminar topic, the study is<br /> case, an inanimate subject occurring where the process<br /> only an extrapolative result found for transitivity and<br /> would normally require an animate one. For example,<br /> modality in a small numbers of the sample. It would<br /> (14) This firm-specific error can control for unobservable firm<br /> be interesting to see whether the tendencies found<br /> effects not captured in the OLS model; (15) According to our<br /> here are corroborated in a larger sample. Also, further<br /> hypothesis, a conflict of interest may exist when a firm with a<br /> research on the other categories of transitivity and<br /> loanoutstandingissuesasecurityunderwrittenbythebank…<br /> modality would be useful./.<br /> There is a rather higher number of examples of active<br /> References:<br /> can in the mental process. Furthermore, one might<br /> add here that the pronoun we as subject, which 1. Berry, M. (1975), Introduction to Systemic Linguistics,<br /> constitutes an unusually high concentration for this Structures and Systems, 1. U.K: Batsford<br /> type of discourse, occurs quite often. Most of the<br /> examples with the pronoun we as subject express 2. Coats, J. (1983), The Semantics of the Modal<br /> legitimacy: (16) We can now state…that… there is no Auxiliaries. London and Canberra: Croom Helm.<br /> evidence…; (17) The second alternative… not only can<br /> account for the increased productivity… 3. Halliday, M.A.K. (1970), Functional diversity in<br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> 68 Số 3 - 9/2016<br /> NGHIÊN CỨU - TRAO ĐỔI v<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> language as seen from a consideration of modality 8. Palmer, F. (1974), The English Verb. U.K.: Longman.<br /> and mood in English. Foundations of Language, 6,<br /> 322-361. 9. Palmer, F. (1986), Mood and Modality. Cambridge<br /> University Press, Cambridge.<br /> 4. Halliday, M.A.K. (1985), An Introduction to Functional<br /> 10. Palmer, F. (1990), Modality and the English Modals.<br /> Grammar. U.K.: Edward Arnold. Huddleston, R.D.<br /> Longman, London and New York.<br /> 5. Huddleston, R.D. (1971), The Sentence in Written<br /> 11. Quirk, R. et al. (1972a), A Comprehensive Grammar<br /> English, a Syntactic Study Based on an Analysis of of the English Language. London and New York:<br /> Scientific Texts, U.K.: Cambridge University Press. Longman.<br /> <br /> 6. Lyons, J. (1977), Semantics. Cambridge University 12. Quirk, R. et al. (1972b), A Grammar of Contemporary<br /> Press, Cambridge. English. London and New York: Lonman.<br /> <br /> 7. McCloskey, D.N. 1985, The Rhetoric of Economics. 13. Von Wright, G.H. (1951), An Essay in Modal Logic.<br /> Harvester Press. Amsterdam: North-Holland Publishing Company.<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> TRANSITIVITY AND MODALITY IN ENGLISH ECONOMIC RESEARCH ARTICLES<br /> <br /> PHAM THI THANH THUY<br /> <br /> Abstract: In the linguistics literature, modality is a semantic term concerning the speaker’s attitude<br /> toward what is being said. Meanwhile, transitivity is the number of objects a verb requires or takes<br /> in a given instance. Much of the attracted attention has been on theoretical nature, and intuitive and<br /> de-contextualized examples. The paper, therefore, helps situate the concept within wider strategies<br /> for modifying illocutionary force basing on an analysis of authentic sources from economic research<br /> articles. Accordingly, the study attempts to further advance research on modal expressions and<br /> transitivity by focusing on analyzing modal verbs and transitivity used in selected economic texts. The<br /> study is based on a corpus of 15 economic research articles drawn from English economic field. The<br /> corpus contains the whole of the text of the articles, excluding the abstract since the language of an<br /> abstract is a register in itself, so it is not like an academic article.<br /> <br /> Keywords: economic research articles, modal verb, transitivity, modality<br /> <br /> Ngày nhận: 18/7/2016<br /> Ngày phản biện: 02/9/2016<br /> Ngày duyệt đăng: 20/9/2016<br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> <br /> KHOA HỌC NGOẠI NGỮ QUÂN SỰ<br /> Số 3 - 9/2016 69<br />
ADSENSE

CÓ THỂ BẠN MUỐN DOWNLOAD

 

Đồng bộ tài khoản
18=>0